Confabulation

· This article is about memory confabulation. For other uses, see Confabulation (Redes neuronales).  Confabulation is a memory disturbance that is characterized by verbal statements or actions that inaccurately describe history, fondo, and present situations.[1] Confabulation is considered “honest lying,” but is distinct from lying because there is typically no intent to deceive and the individuals are unaware that their information is false.[2] Although patients can present blatantly false information (“fantastic confabulation”), confabulatory information can also be coherent, internally consistent, and relatively normal.[2] Individuals who confabulate are generally very confident about their recollections, despite evidence contradicting its truthfulness.[3] The most known causes of confabulation are traumatic and acquired (p. ej.., aneurysm, edema) brain damage, and psychiatric or psychological disorders (p. ej.., esquizofrenia, bipolar, Alzheimer`s).  Two distinct types of confabulation are often distinguished: spontaneous and provoked. Spontaneous, or primary, confabulations do not occur in response to a cue[4] and seem to be involuntary.[5] Spontaneous confabulation is also relatively rare and may result from the interaction between frontal lobe pathology and organic amnesia, and is more common in cases of dementia.[6] Provoked, momentary, or secondary, confabulation represents a normal response to a faulty memory and is common in both amnesia and dementia.[6] Provoked confabulations can become apparent during memory tests.[4] Another distinction found in confabulations is that between verbal and behavioral. Verbal confabulations are spoken false memories and are more common, while behavioral confabulations occur when an individual acts on their false memories.[5] Confabulated memories of all types most often occur in autobiographical memory, and are indicative of a complicated and intricate process that can be led astray at any point during encoding, storage, or recall of a memory.[3] This type of confabulation is commonly seen in Korsakoff's syndrome.[7] Contenido 1 Characteristic features 2 Organic causes 3 Psychological causes 4 teorías 4.1 Neuropsychological theories 4.2 Self-Identity Theory 4.3 Temporality Theory 4.4 Monitoring Theory 4.5 Strategic Retrieval Account Theory 4.6 Executive Control Theory 4.7 Confabulation in the Context of Delusion Theories 4.8 Fuzzy-Trace Theory 4.9 Epistemic Theory 5 Presentación 5.1 Neurological and Psychological Conditions Associated with Confabulation 5.2 Location of Brain Lesions Linked to Confabulation 5.3 Developmental Differences in Confabulation 5.4 Provoked versus Spontaneous Confabulations 5.5 Confidence in False Memories 5.6 Confabulation Among Normal Subjects 6 Diagnosis and treatment 6.1 Deese-Roediger-McDermott Lists 6.2 Recognition Tasks 6.3 Free Recall Tasks 6.4 Tratamiento 7 Investigar 8 Ver también 9 Referencias 10 Further reading Characteristic features Typically verbal statements but can also be non-verbal gestures or actions. Can include autobiographical and non-personal information, such as historical facts, fairytales, or other aspects of semantic memory. The account can be fantastic or coherent. Both the premise and the details of the account can be false. The account is usually drawn from the patient’s memory of actual experiences, including past and current thoughts. The patient is unaware of the accountsdistortions or inappropriateness, and is not concerned when errors are pointed out. There is no hidden motivation behind the account. The patient’s personality structure may play a role in their readiness to confabulate. [2] Organic causes Berlyne (1972) defined confabulation as “a falsification of memory occurring in clear consciousness in association with an organically derived amnesia.He distinguished between: “momentary” (or “provoked”) confabulations - fleeting, and invariably provoked by questions probing the subject’s memorysometimes consisting of “realmemories displaced in their temporal context. “fantastic” (or “spontaneous”) confabulations - characterised by the spontaneous outpouring of irrelevant associationssometimes bizarre ideas, which may be held with firm conviction. Patients who have suffered brain damage or lesions, especially to the Prefrontal cortical regions, may have confabulation of memories as a symptom. Patients with Korsakoff's syndrome characteristically confabulate by guessing an answer or imagining an event and then mistaking their guess or imagination for an actual memory. En algunos casos, confabulation is a function of the brain's chemistry, a mapping of the activation of neurons to brain activity. [8] Confabulation can also occur as a result of damage to the Anterior communicating artery (ACoA), in the Circle of Willis. Some military agents, such as BZ, and deliriant drugs such as those found in datura, noticeably scopolamine and atropine, may also cause confabulation. Psychological causes Bartlett’s [9] studies of remembering are arguably the first concerted attempt to look at memory illusions phenomena. en un experimento, he asked a group of students to read an Indian folktale and then recall that at various time intervals. As well as errors of omission, interestingly he found numerous errors of commission whereby participants had adapted or added to the story to make it more rational or consistent. In the 1970s a number of researchers and theories started to emphasise what has been called the constructivist view of memory, maintaining that reasoning influences memory, in contrast to the prevailing view at the time which was that memory is essential for proper reasoning [10]. Theorists such as Bransford and Franks [11] noted the significance of personal beliefs and desires, or more technically scripts and schemas, in memory retrieval. Constructivism has fallen out of fashion recently due to the contention that it is either false or un-testable [12]. Memory is presumably not always reconstructive as the considerable evidence of its veridical quality is testament. Constructivism cannot simply be rephrased as the thesis that memory is not always reproductive. As Reyna and Lloyd[12] point out, this amounts to the claim that memory is sometimes reproductive and sometimes reconstructive; which is unexplanatory and unfalsefiable as any result can be accommodated post hoc. Because of this a number of theories have now been advanced which instead focus on the mechanism by which an essentially accurate memory system can sometimes produce erroneous results. Notably, both source monitoring framework [13] and fuzzy-trace theory [10] purport to both indicate when false memories are likely to occur and give a more detailed explanatory account than either reproductive or constructivist views. Source monitoring refers to the process by which we discriminate between internally and externally derived memory sources as well as differentiations within the external and external domains: differentiating between two external sources or between internal sources, for instance between what was said and what was thought. The theory postulates that these decisions are made based on the characteristics of memories compared to norms for memories for different sources, such as the proportions of perceptual, contextual, affective and semantic information featured in the encoding of the memory. Under the source monitoring framework false memory is seen as a failure to attribute information to the correct source. This happens when there is insufficient information available to discriminate between different sources (perhaps because of natural deterioration), or when the wrong criterion is used to discriminate. For example a doctor might mistakenly think a patient is on a specific medicine because they were discussing the medicine with a colleague shortly after seeing them. Fuzzy trace theory is based on the assumption that memory is not stored in unitary form. Instead memories are encoded on a number of levels, from an exact ‘verbatimaccount, to ‘gistwhich represents the overall meaning of the event[12]. False memory effects are usually (pero no siempre) explained as a reliance on gist traces in a situation when verbatim traces are needed. Because of this people may mistakenly recall a memory that only goes along with a vague gist of what happened, rather than the exact course of events. Essentially there are three reasons why people might do this. There is thought to be a general bias towards the use of gist traces in cognition due to their resource efficiency [10] and people will tend to use gist traces when it is thought that they will be adequate to satisfy the demands of the situation. Segundo, verbatim traces are said to be inherently less stable than gist and decay quicker [10]. Finalmente, during the course of forgetting memories fragment and gist and verbatim can become independent[12].  Theories Theories of confabulation range in emphasis. Some theories propose that confabulations represent a way for memory-disabled individuals to maintain their self-identity.[4] Other theories use neurocognitive links to explain the process of confabulation.[14] Still other theories frame confabulation around the more familiar concept of delusion.[15] Other researchers frame confabulation within the fuzzy-trace theory.[16] Finalmente, some researchers call for theories that rely less on neurocognitive explanations and more on epistemic accounts.[17] Neuropsychological theories The most popular theories of confabulation come from the field of neuropsychology or cognitive neuroscience.[14] Research suggests that confabulation is associated with dysfunction of cognitive processes that control the retrieval from long-term memory. Frontal lobe damage often disrupts this process, preventing the retrieval of information and the evaluation of its output.[18][19] Además, researchers argue that confabulation is a disorder resulting from failed “reality monitoring/source monitoring” (es decir. deciding whether a memory is based on an actual event or whether it is imagined.[20] Some neuropsychologists suggest that errors in retrieval of information from long-term memory that are made by normal subjects involve different components of control processes than errors made by confabulators.[21] Detection of these errors are considered part of the Supervisory System,[22] which is believed to be a function of the frontal cortex. Self-Identity Theory Some argue confabulations have a self-serving, emotional component in those with memory deficits that aids to maintain a coherent, self-concept.[4] En otras palabras., individuals who confabulate are motivated to do so, because they have gaps in their memory that they want to fill in and cover up. Temporality Theory Support for the temporality account suggests that confabulations occur when an individual is unable to place events properly in time.[4] Así, an individual might correctly state an action they performed, but say they did it yesterday, when they did it weeks ago. In the Memory, Consciousness, and Temporality Theory, confabulation occurs because of a deficit in temporal consciousness or awareness.[23] Monitoring Theory Along a similar notion are the theories of reality and source monitoring theories.[5] In these theories, confabulation occurs when individuals incorrectly attribute memories as reality, or incorrectly attribute memories to a certain source. Así, an individual might claim an imagined event happened in reality, or that their friend told them about an event, they actually heard about on television. Strategic Retrieval Account Theory Supporters of the strategic retrieval account suggest that confabulations occur when an individual cannot actively monitor a memory for truthfulness after its retrieval.[5] An individual recalls a memory, but there is some deficit after recall that interferes with the person establishing its falseness. Executive Control Theory Still others propose that all types of false memories, including confabulation, fit into a general memory and executive function model.[24] En 2007, a framework for confabulation was proposed that stated confabulation is the result of two things: problems with executive control and problems with evaluation. In the executive control deficit, the incorrect memory is retrieved from the brain. In the evaluative deficit, the memory will be accepted as a truth due to an inability to distinguish a belief from an actual memory.[4] Confabulation in the Context of Delusion Theories Recent models of confabulation have attempted to build upon the link between delusion and confabulation.[15] Más recientemente, a monitoring account for delusion, applied to confabulation, proposed both the inclusion of conscious and unconscious processing. The claim was that by encompassing the notion of both processes, spontaneous versus provoked confabulations could be better explained. En otras palabras., there are two ways to confabulate. One is the unconscious, spontaneous way in which a memory goes through no logical, explanatory processing. The other is the conscious, provoked way in which a memory is recalled intentionally by the individual to explain something confusing or unusual.[25] Fuzzy-Trace Theory Fuzzy-trace theory, or FTT, is a concept more commonly applied to the explanation of judgment decisions.[16] According to this theory, memories are encoded generally (gist), as well as specifically (verbatim). Así, a confabulation could result from recalling the incorrect verbatim memory or from being able to recall the gist portion, but not the verbatim portion, of a memory. FTT uses a set of five principles to explain false-memory phenomena. Principle 1 suggests that subjects store verbatim information and gist information parallel to one another. Both forms of storage involve the surface content of an experience. Principle 2 shares factors of retrieval of gist and verbatim traces. Principle 3 is based on dual-opponent processes in false memory. En general, gist retrieval supports false memory, while verbatim retrieval suppresses it. Developmental variability is the topic of Principle 4. As a child develops into an adult, there is obvious improvement in the acquisition, retention, and retrieval of both verbatim and gist memory. Sin embargo, during late adulthood, there will be a decline in these abilities. Finalmente, Principle 5 explains that verbatim and gist processing cause vivid remembering. Fuzzy-trace Theory, governed by these 5 principles, has proved useful in explaining false memory and generating new predictions about it.[26] Epistemic Theory However, not all accounts are so embedded in the neurocognitive aspects of confabulation. Some attribute confabulation to epistemic accounts.[17] En 2009, theories underlying the causation and mechanisms for confabulation were criticized for their focus on neural processes, which are somewhat unclear, as well as their emphasis on the negativity of false remembering. Researchers proposed that an epistemic account of confabulation would be more encompassing of both the advantages and disadvantages of the process. Presentation Neurological and Psychological Conditions Associated with Confabulation Confabulations are often symptoms of various syndromes and psychopathologies in the adult population including: Korsakoff's syndrome, Alzheimer’s Disease, Esquizofrenia, and traumatic brain injury. Wernicke-Korsakoff syndrome is a neurological disorder typically characterized by years of chronic alcohol abuse and a nutritional thiamine deficiency.[27] Confabulation is one salient symptom of this syndrome.[28][29] A study on confabulation in Korsakoff’s patients found that they are subject to provoked confabulation when prompted with questions pertaining to episodic memory, not semantic memory, and when prompted with questions where the appropriate response would be “I don’t know.”[30] This suggests that confabulation in these patients is “domain-specific.Korsakoff’s patients who confabulate are more likely than healthy adults to falsely recognize distractor words, suggesting that false recognition is a “confabulatory behavior.Alzheimer’s Disease is a condition with both neurological and psychological components. It is a form of dementia associated with severe frontal lobe dysfunction. Confabulation in individuals with Alzheimer’s is often more spontaneous than it is in other conditions, especially in the advanced stages of the disease. Alzheimer’s patients demonstrate comparable abilities to encode information as healthy elderly adults, suggesting that impairments in encoding are not associated with confabulation.[31] Sin embargo, as seen in Korsakoff's patients, confabulation in Alzheimer’s patients is higher when prompted with questions investigating episodic memory. Researchers suggest this is due to damage in the posterior cortical regions of the brain, which is a symptom characteristic of Alzheimer’s Disease. Schizophrenia is a psychological disorder in which confabulation is sometimes observed. Although confabulation is usually coherent in its presentation, confabulations of schizophrenic patients are often delusional[32] Researchers have noted that these patients tend to make up delusions on the spot which are often fantastic and become increasingly elaborate with questioning.[33] Unlike patients with Korsakoff's and Alzheimer's, patients with schizophrenia are more likely to confabulate when prompted with questions regarding their semantic memories, as opposed to episodic memory prompting.[34] Además, confabulation does not appear to be related to any memory deficit in schizophrenic patients. This is contrary to most forms of confabulation. Además, confabulations made by schizophrenic patients often do not involve the creation of new information, but instead involve an attempt by the patient to reconstruct actual details of a past event. Traumatic brain injury (TBI) can also result in confabulation. Research has shown that patients with damage to the inferior medial frontal lobe confabulate significantly more than patients with damage to the posterior area and healthy controls.[35] This suggests that this region is key in producing confabulatory responses, and that memory deficit is important but not necessary in confabulation. Adicionalmente, research suggests that confabulation can be seen in patients with frontal lobe syndrome, which involves an insult to the frontal lobe as a result of disease or traumatic brain injury (TBI).[36][37] Finalmente, rupture of the anterior or posterior communicating artery, subarachnoid hemorrhage, and encephalitis are also possible causes of confabulation.[1][38] Location of Brain Lesions Linked to Confabulation Confabulation is believed to be a result of damage to the right frontal lobe of the brain.[2] En particular, damage can be localized to the ventromedial frontal lobes and other structures fed by the anterior communicating artery (ACoA), including the basal forebrain, septum, fornix, cingulate gyrus, cingulum, anterior hypothalamus, and head of the caudate nucleus.[39][40] Developmental Differences in Confabulation While some recent literature has suggested that older adults may be more susceptible than their younger counterparts to have false memories, the majority of research on forced confabulation centers around children.[41] Children are particularly susceptible to forced confabulations based on their high suggestibility.[42][43] When forced to recall confabulated events, children are less likely to remember that they had previously confabulated these situations, and they are more likely than their adult counterparts to come to remember these confabulations as real events that transpired.[44] Research suggests that this inability to distinguish between past confabulatory and real events is centered on developmental differences in source monitoring. Due to underdeveloped encoding and critical reasoning skills, children's ability to distinguish real memories from false memories may be impaired. It may also be that younger children lack the meta-memory processes required to remember confabulated versus non-confabulated events.[45] Children's meta-memory processes may also be influenced by expectancies or biases, in that they believe that highly plausible false scenarios are not confabulated.[46] Sin embargo, when knowingly being tested for accuracy, children are more likely to respond, “I don’t knowat a rate comparable to adults for unanswerable questions than they are to confabulate.[47][48] Finalmente, misinformation effects can be minimized by tailoring individual interviews to the specific developmental stage, often based on age, of the participant.[49] Provoked versus Spontaneous Confabulations There is evidence to support different cognitive mechanisms for provoked and spontaneous confabulation.[50] One study suggested that spontaneous confabulation may be a result of an amnesic patient’s inability to distinguish the chronological order of events in his memory. En contraste, provoked confabulation may be a compensatory mechanism, in which the patient tries to make up for his memory deficiency by attempting to demonstrate competency in recollection. Confidence in False Memories Confabulation of events or situations may lead to an eventual acceptance of the confabulated information as true.[51] Por ejemplo, people who knowingly lie about a situation may eventually come to believe that their lies are truthful with time.[52] In an interview setting, people are more likely to confabulate in situations in which they are presented false information by another person, as opposed to when they self-generate these falsehoods.[53] Más lejos, people are more likely to accept false information as true when they are interviewed at a later time (after the event in question) than those who are interviewed immediately or soon after the event.[54] Affirmative feedback for confabulated responses is also shown to increase the confabulator’s confidence in their response.[55] Por ejemplo, in culprit identification, if a witness falsely identifies a member of a line-up, he will be more confident in his identification if the interviewer provides affirmative feedback. This effect of confirmatory feedback appears to last over time, as witnesses will even remember the confabulated information months later.[56] Confabulation Among Normal Subjects On rare occasions, confabulation can also be seen in normal subjects.[21] It is currently unclear how completely healthy individuals produce confabulations. It is possible that these individuals are in the process of developing some type of organic condition that is causing their confabulation symptoms. It is not uncommon, sin embargo, for the general population to display some very mild symptoms of provoked confabulations. Subtle distortions and intrusions in memory are commonly produced by normal subjects when they remember something poorly. Diagnosis and treatment Spontaneous confabulations, due to their involuntary nature, cannot be manipulated in a laboratory setting.[5] Sin embargo, provoked confabulations can be researched in various theoretical contexts. The mechanisms found to underlie provoked confabulations can be applied to spontaneous confabulation mechanisms. The basic premise of researching confabulation comprises finding errors and distortions in memory tests of an individual. Deese-Roediger-McDermott Lists Confabulations can be detected in the context of the Deese-Roediger-McDermott paradigm by using the Deese-Roediger-McDermott lists.[57] Participants listen to audio recordings of several lists of words centered around a theme, known as the critical word. The participants are later asked to recall the words on their list. If the participant recalls the critical word, which was never explicitly stated in the list, it is considered a confabulation. Participants often have a false memory for the critical word. Recognition Tasks Confabulations can also be researched by using continuous recognition tasks.[5] These tasks are often used in conjunction with confidence ratings. En general, in a recognition task, participants are rapidly presented with pictures. Some of these pictures are shown once; others are shown multiple times. Participants press a key if they have seen the picture previously. Following a period of time, participants repeat the task. More errors on the second task, versus the first, are indicative of confusion, representing false memories. Free Recall Tasks Confabulations can also be detected using a free recall task, such as a self-narrative task.[5] Participants are asked to recall stories (semantic or autobiographical) that are highly familiar to them. The stories recalled are encoded for errors that could be classified as distortions in memory. Distortions could include falsifying true story elements or including details from a completely different story. Errors such as these would be indicative of confabulations. Treatment Treatment for confabulation is somewhat dependent on the cause or source, if identifiable. Por ejemplo, treatment of Wernicke-Korsakoff syndrome involves large doses of vitamin B in order to reverse the thiamine deficiency.[58] If there is no known physiological cause, more general cognitive techniques may be used to treat confabulation. In a recent case study, Self-Monitoring Training (SMT)[59] was shown to reduce delusional confabulations. Además, improvements were maintained at a three-month follow-up and were found to generalize to everyday settings. Although this treatment seems promising, more rigorous research is necessary to determine its efficacy in the general confabulation population. Research Although significant gains have been made in the understanding of confabulation within recent years, there is still much to be learned. One group of researchers in particular has laid out several important questions for future study.[60] They suggest that more information is necessary regarding the neural systems that support the different cognitive processes needed for normal source monitoring. They also proposed the idea of developing a standard neuropsychological test battery that is able to discriminate between the different types of confabulations. Además, there is a considerable amount of debate regarding the best way to organize and combine neuroimaging, pharmacological, and cognitive/behavioral approaches to understand confabulation. In a recent review article, another group of researchers[61] contemplate issues concerning the distinctions between delusions and confabulation. They question whether delusions and confabulation should be considered distinct or overlapping disorders and, if overlapping, to what degree? They also discuss the role of unconscious processes in confabulation. Some researchers suggest that unconscious emotional and motivational processes are potentially just as important as cognitive and memory problems. Finalmente, they raise the question of where to draw the line between the pathological and the nonpathological. Delusion-like beliefs and confabulation-like fabrications are commonly seen in healthy individuals. What are the important differences between patients with similar etiology who do and do not confabulate? Since the line between pathological and nonpathological is likely blurry, should we take a more dimensional approach to confabulation? Research suggests that confabulation occurs along a continuum of implausibility, bizarreness, content, conviction, preoccupation, and distress, and impact on daily life. See also Anosognosia Anton-Babinski syndrome Cryptamnesia Déjà vu False memory Korsakoffs psychosis ReferencesJump up to: 1.0 1.1 Dalla Barba, G. (1993). Confabulation: knowledge and recollective experience. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 10(1), 1-20. ↑ Jump up to: 2.0 2.1 2.2 2.3 Moscovitch M. 1995. Confabulation. En (Eds. Schacter D.L., Coyle J.T., Fischbach G.D., Mesulum M.M. & Sullivan L.G.), Memory Distortion (páginas. 226-251). Cambridge, MAMÁ: Prensa de la Universidad de Harvard. ↑ Jump up to: 3.0 3.1 Nalbantian, edited by Suzanne; matthews,, Paul M., McClelland, Santiago L. (2010). The memory process: neuroscientific and humanistic perspectives, Cambridge, Mass.: Prensa del MIT. ↑ Jump up to: 4.0 4.1 4.2 4.3 4.4 4.5 Metcalf, Kasey, Langdon, Robyn, Coltheart, Max (1 Febrero 2007). Models of confabulation: A critical review and a new framework. Cognitive Neuropsychology 24 (1): 23–47. ↑ Jump up to: 5.0 5.1 5.2 5.3 5.4 5.5 5.6 Gilboa, Un. (13 Abril 2006). Mechanisms of spontaneous confabulations: a strategic retrieval account. Cerebro 129 (6): 1399–1414. ↑ Jump up to: 6.0 6.1 Kopelman, M.D.. Two types of confabulation. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1987a; 50: 1482–7. ↑ D. Kopelman, Miguel, Allan D. Thomson,, Irene Guerrini, E. Jane Marshall. The Korsakoff Syndrome: Clinical Aspects, Psychology and Treatment. oxford journals 44 (2): 148–154. ↑ Confabulation theory - ScholarpediaBartlett, F., Remembering: a study in experimental and social psychology, Cambridge, Inglaterra: Cambridge University, 1932 ↑ Jump up to: 10.0 10.1 10.2 10.3 Reyna, V. F. & Brainerd, C. J., Fuzzy trace theory: an interim synthesis. Learning and individual differences, 7, 1-75, 1995 ↑ Bransford, J. D. & francos, J., The abstraction of linguistic ideas. Psicología cognitiva, 2, 331-350., 1971 ↑ Jump up to: 12.0 12.1 12.2 12.3 Reyna, V. F. & lloyd, F., Theories of false memory in children and adults. Learning individual differences, 9 (2), 95-123, 1997 ↑ Johnson, Hashtroudi & Lindsay, Source monitoring. Boletín Psicológico, 114, 3-28, 1993 ↑ Jump up to: 14.0 14.1 Glowinski, Remy, Payman, Vahid, Frencham, Kate (1 Enero 2008). Confabulation: a spontaneous and fantastic review. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 42 (11): 932–940. ↑ Jump up to: 15.0 15.1 Kopelman, miguel d. (1 Enero 2010). Varieties of confabulation and delusion. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 15 (1-3): 14–37. ↑ Jump up to: 16.0 16.1 Brainerd, cj, Reyna, V.F. (1 Noviembre 1998). Fuzzy-Trace Theory and Children's False Memories. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 71 (2): 81–129. ↑ Jump up to: 17.0 17.1 Bortolotti, Lisa, Timonel, Rochelle E. (1 Diciembre 2009). ‘Faultlessignorance: Strengths and limitations of epistemic definitions of confabulation. Consciousness and Cognition 18 (4): 952–965. ↑ Baddeley, D.C. & wilson, B. (1986) Amnesia, autobiographical memory and confabulation., (Editor): Frotar, D.C In: Autobiographical Memory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, p. 225 - 252. ↑ Moscovitch M., Melo B. (1997). Strategic retrieval and the frontal lobes: evidence from confabulation and amnesia. Neuropsicología 35: 1017–1034. ↑ Johnson, M.K. (1991). Reality monitoring: Evidence from confabulation in organic brain disease patients. In G.P. Prigatano & D.L. Schacter (Eds.), Awareness of deficit after brain injury (páginas. 176-197). Nueva York: Oxford. ↑ Jump up to: 21.0 21.1 Burgess, P. W. and Shallice, T. (1996) Confabulation and the control of recollection. Memoria 4, 359-411. ↑ Norman, DA, & Shallice, T. (1980). Attention to action. Willed and automatic control of behavior. University of California San Diego CHIP Report 99. ↑ Dalla Barba, Gianfranco, Boissé, Marie-Françoise (1 Enero 2010). Temporal consciousness and confabulation: Is the medial temporal lobe "temporal"?. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 15 (1-3): 95–117. ↑ Kopelman, miguel d. (1 Mayo 1999). VARIETIES OF FALSE MEMORY. Cognitive Neuropsychology 16 (3-5): 197–214. ↑ Tornero, Martha, Coltheart, Max (1 Enero 2010). Confabulation and delusion: A common monitoring framework. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 15 (1-3): 346–376. ↑ Error de cita: No válido etiqueta; no text was provided for refs named Fuzzy-Trace Theory and False MemoryHomewood, J., & Bond, N. W. (1999). Thiamin deficiency and Korsakoff’s syndrome: Failure to find memory impairments following nonalcoholic Wernicke’s encephalopathy. Alcohol, 19, 75–84. ↑ Dalla Barba, G., Cipolotti, L., & Denes, G. (1990). Autobiographical memory loss and confabulation in Korsakoff’s syndrome: Un informe de caso. Córtex, 26, 525–534. ↑ Kessels RP, Kortrijk HE, Wester AJ, Nys GM. Confabulation behavior and false memories in Korsakoff's syndrome: role of source memory and executive functioning. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2008 Apr;62(2):220-5. ↑ Damme, Ilse, d'Ydewalle, Géry (2010). Confabulation versus experimentally induced false memories in Korsakoff patients. Journal of Neuropsychology 4 (2): 211–230. ↑ Tonelero, Janine M., Shanks, Michael F., Venneri, Annalena (11 Mayo 2006). Provoked confabulations in Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsicología 44 (10): 1697–1707. ↑ Wing, J. K., Tonelero, J. E., Sartorius, N. 1974. The description and classification of psychiatric symptoms: An instruction manual for the PSE and catego system. Londres: Cambridge University Press. ↑ Lorente-Rovira , E. , Pomarol-Clotet , E. , McCarthy , REAL ACADEMIA DE BELLAS ARTES. , Berríos, G.E. , & McKenna , P.J.. ( 2007 ). Confabulation in schizophrenia and its relationship to clinical and neuropsychological features of the disorder . Medicina Psicológica , 1 – 10. ↑ LORENTE-ROVIRA, E., SANTOS-GÓMEZ, J.L., MORO, M., VILLAGRÁN, JM, MCKENNA, P.J.. (1 Noviembre 2010). Confabulation in schizophrenia: A neuropsychological study. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 16 (06): 1018–1026. ↑ Tornero, Martha S., Cipolotti, Lisa, Yousry, Tarek A., Shallice, Tim (1 Junio 2008). Confabulation: Damage to a specific inferior medial prefrontal system. Córtex 44 (6): 637–648. ↑ Baddeley, D.C.. &wilson, B. (1988). Frontal amnesia and the dysexecutive syndrome. Brain and Cognition, 7, 212–230. ↑ Papagno, C. & Baddeley, D.C.. (1997) Confabulation in a dysexecutive patient: Implications for models of retrieval. Córtex, 33, 743-752. Baddeley, D.C.. (1996). Exploring the Central Executive. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49Un (1): 5-28. ↑ Baddeley, D.C. & wilson, B. (1986) Amnesia, autobiographical memory and confabulation., (Editor): Frotar, D.C In: Autobiographical Memory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, p. 225 - 252. ↑ Alexander MP, Freedman M. Amnesia after anterior communicating artery aneurysm rupture. Neurología 1984; 34: 752–7. ↑ Irle E, Wowra B, Kunert HJ, Hampl J, Kunze S. Memory disturbances following anterior communicating artery rupture. Ann Neurol 1992; 31: 473–80. ↑ Brainerd, C. J., Reyna, V. F., Ceci, S. J. (2008). Developmental reversals in false memory: A review of data and theory.. Boletín Psicológico 134 (3): 343–382. ↑ Shapiro, Lauren R., Purdy, Telisa L. (2005). Suggestibility and source monitoring errors: blame the interview style, interviewer consistency, and the child's personality. Applied Cognitive Psychology 19 (4): 489–506. ↑ Shapiro, Lauren R., Blackford, Cheryl, Chen, Chiung-Fen (2005). Eyewitness memory for a simulated misdemeanor crime: the role of age and temperament in suggestibility. Applied Cognitive Psychology 19 (3): 267–289. ↑ Ackil, Jennifer K., Zaragoza, Maria S. (1 Noviembre 1998). Consecuencias conmemorativas de la confabulación forzada: Age differences in susceptibility to false memories.. Psicología del desarrollo 34 (6): 1358–1372. ↑ Ghetti, Simona, Castelli, Paola, lyon, Kristen E. (2010). Knowing about not remembering: developmental dissociations in lack-of-memory monitoring. Developmental Science 13 (4): 611–621. ↑ Ghetti, Simona, Alexander, Kristen Weede (2004). "If It Happened, I Would Remember It": Strategic Use of Event Memorability in the Rejection of False Autobiographical Events. Desarrollo del niño 75 (2): 542–561. ↑ Roebers, Claudia, Fernández, Olivia (2002). The Effects of Accuracy Motivation on Children's and Adults' Event Recall, Suggestibility, and Their Answers to Unanswerable Questions. Journal of Cognition and Development 3 (4): 415–443. ↑ Scoboria, Alan, Mazzoni, Giuliana, kirsch, Irving (2008). "Don't know" responding to answerable and unanswerable questions during misleading and hypnotic interviews.. Revista de Psicología Experimental: Applied 14 (3): 255–265. ↑ Holliday, Robyn E., Albon, Amanda J. (2004). Minimising misinformation effects in young children with cognitive interview mnemonics. Applied Cognitive Psychology 18 (3): 263–281. ↑ Schnider, Armin, von Däniken, Christine, Gutbrod, Klemens (19 Febrero 1996). The mechanisms of spontaneous and provoked confabulations. Cerebro 119 (4): 1365–1375. ↑ Pickel, Kerri (2004). When a lie becomes the truth: The effects of selfgenerated misinformation on eyewitness memory. Memoria 12 (1): 14–26. ↑ Polage, Danielle C. (2004). Fabrication deflation? The mixed effects of lying on memory. Applied Cognitive Psychology 18 (4): 455–465. ↑ Pezdek, Kathy, Lam, Shirley T., Sperry, Kathryn (2009). Forced confabulation more strongly influences event memory if suggestions are other-generated than self-generated. Legal and Criminological Psychology 14 (2): 241–252. ↑ Melnyk, Laura, Bruck, Maggie (2004). Timing moderates the effects of repeated suggestive interviewing on children's eyewitness memory. Applied Cognitive Psychology 18 (5): 613–631. ↑ Hafstad, Gertrud Sofie, Memon, Amina, Logie, Robert (2004). Post-identification feedback, confidence and recollections of witnessing conditions in child witnesses. Applied Cognitive Psychology 18 (7): 901–912. ↑ Zaragoza, M. S., Payment, K. E., Ackil, J. K., Drivdahl, S. B., Beck, M. (2001). Interviewing Witnesses: Forced Confabulation and Confirmatory Feedback Increase False Memories. Psychological Science 12 (6): 473–477. ↑ Howe, Mark L., Cicchetti, Dante, Toth, Sheree L., Cerrito, Beth M. (1 Septiembre 2004). True and False Memories in Maltreated Children. Desarrollo del niño 75 (5): 1402–1417. ↑ Spiegel, D., & Lim, KJ. 2011. A Case of Probable Korsakoff's Syndrome: A Syndrome of Frontal Lobe and Diencephalic Structural Pathogenesis and a Comparison with Medial Temporal Lobe Dementias. Innov Clin Neurosci. 2011 Junio; 8(6): 15–19. ↑ B. Dayus and M.D. van den Broek. 2000. Treatment of stable delusional confabulations using self-monitoring training. NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL REHABILITATION, 10 (4), 415–427. ↑ Johnson, M. K., & Raye, C. L. (1998). False memories and confabulation: Trends in Cognitive Sciences Vol 2(4) Apr 1998, 137-145. ↑ Langdon, R. & Tornero, M (Enero 2010), "Delusion and confabulation: Overlapping or distinct distortions in reality?", Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 15 (1): 1–13, PMID 20043251 Hirstein, William (2004). Brain Fiction: Self-Deception and the Riddle of Confabulation, La prensa del MIT. ISBN 978-0-262-58271-1. Kalat, J. W., (2002). Biological Psychology (8El d). toronto, Ontario, Canadá: Thomson Wadsworth. Stedman, T. L. (2000, Enero 15). Stedman's Medical Dictionary (27ª ed.). Filadelfia, PAPÁ: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Further reading Amnestic-confabulatory syndrome in hydrocephalic dementia and Korsakoff's psychosis in alcoholism. (1979). Acta psiquiátrica Scandinavica vol. 60(4) Oct 1979, 323-333. Ackil, J. K., & Zaragoza, M. S. (1998). Consecuencias conmemorativas de la confabulación forzada: Diferencias de edad en la susceptibilidad a los falsos recuerdos: Psicología del desarrollo vol. 34(6) Nov 1998, 1358-1372. Anastasi, J. S. (2006). Understanding confabulation: A multidisciplinary approach: Psicología Cognitiva Aplicada Vol. 20(2) Estropear 2006, 277-278. Baddeley, Un., & wilson, B. (1986). Amnesia, autobiographical memory, and confabulation. Nueva York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Barba, G. D. (1993). Different patterns of confabulation: Cortex Vol 29(4) Dic 1993, 567-581. Baxter, J. S. (1990). Children as eyewitnesses: Un estudio de desarrollo: Resúmenes de disertación internacional. benson, D. F., Djenderedjian, Un., Molinero, B. L., Pachana, N. Un., & et al. (1996). Neural basis of confabulation: Neurología vol. 46(5) Mayo 1996, 1239-1243. Bergman, E. T., & Roediger, H. L., III. (1999). Can Bartlett's repeated reproduction experiments be replicated? : Memoria & Cognition Vol 27(6) Nov 1999, 937-947. Berríos, G. E. (1998). Confabulations: A conceptual history: Journal of the History of the Neurosciences Vol 7(3) Dic 1998, 225-241. Berríos, G. E. (2000). Confabulations. Nueva York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Bickle, J. (2005). Review of Brain Fiction: Journal of Consciousness Studies Vol 12(7) Jul 2005, 87-88. Birnberg, J. D. (2000). The ecology of confabulation: Deviant responses in the Rorschach sequence. Resúmenes de disertación internacional: Sección B: Las Ciencias y la Ingeniería. Blechner, M. J. (2000). Confabulation in dreaming, psicosis, and brain injury: Comentario: Neuro-Psychoanalysis Vol 2(2) 2000, 139-144. Blechner, M. J. (2004). comentario sobre "The Pleasantness of False Beliefs": Neuro-Psychoanalysis Vol 6(1) 2004, 16-20. Blechner, M. J. (2007). "Confabulation in dementia: Constantly compensating memory systems": Comentario: Neuro-Psychoanalysis Vol 9(1) 2007, 17-22. Caja, O., Laing, H., & Kopelman, M. (1999). The evolution of spontaneous confabulation, delusional misidentification and a related delusion in a case of severe head injury: Neurocase Vol 5(3) 1999, 251-262. Brodski, S. L. (1999). Confabulations. Washington, DC: Asociación Americana de Psicología. Brugger, PAGS., & Funk, M. (2007). Out on a limb: Neglect and confabulation in the study of aplasic phantoms. Nueva York, NY: Oxford University Press. Buck, J. Un., Conejera, Un. R., Betman, S. Yo., & Brigham, J. C. (2002). Age differences in Criteria-Based Content Analysis scores in typical child sexual abuse interviews: Revista de Psicología del Desarrollo Aplicada Vol. 23(3) Mayo-Junio 2002, 267-283. Burgess, P. W., & Shallice, T. (1996). Confabulation and the Control of Recollection: Memory Vol 4(4) Jul 1996, 359-411. Canestri, J. (2000). "A cognitive neuroscience perspective on confabulation": Comentario: Neuro-Psychoanalysis Vol 2(2) 2000, 144-148. Ciaramelli, E., & Ghetti, S. (2007). What are confabulators' memories made of? A study of subjective and objective measures of recollection in confabulation: Neuropsicología Vol. 45(7) 2007, 1489-1500. Ciaramelli, E., Ghetti, S., Frattarelli, M., & Ladavas, E. (2006). When true memory availability promotes false memory: Evidence from confabulating patients: Neuropsicología Vol. 44(10) 2006, 1866-1877. Clare, Yo. C., & Gudjonson, G. H. (1993). Interrogative suggestibility, confabulation, and acquiescence in people with mild learning disabilities (mental handicap): Implications for reliability during police interrogations: British Journal of Clinical Psychology Vol 32(3) Sep 1993, 295-301. Tonelero, J. M., Shanks, M. F., & Venneri, Un. (2006). Provoked confabulations in Alzheimer's disease: Neuropsicología Vol. 44(10) 2006, 1697-1707. Cunningham, J. M. (1996). The contribution of memory deficits and executive dysfunction to the production of confabulation. Resúmenes de disertación internacional: Sección B: Las Ciencias y la Ingeniería. Cunningham, J. M., Pliskin, N. H., Cassisi, J. E., Tsang, B., & Rao, S. M. (1997). Relationship between confabulation and measures of memory and executive function: Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology Vol 19(6) Dic 1997, 867-877. Dab, S., Claes, T., Morais, J., & Shallice, T. (1999). Confabulation with a selective descriptor process impairment: Cognitive Neuropsychology Vol 16(3-5) May-Jul 1999, 215-242. Dab, S., Morais, J., & Frith, C. (2004). Comprehension, encoding, and monitoring in the production of confabulation in memory: A study with schizophrenic patients: Cognitive Neuropsychiatry Vol 9(3) Ago 2004, 153-182. Dalla Barba, G. (1993). Confabulation: Knowledge and recollective experience: Cognitive Neuropsychology Vol 10(1) Feb 1993, 1-20. Dalla Barba, G. (1995). Consciousness and confabulation: Remembering 'another' past. Malden, MAMÁ: Publicaciones de Blackwell. Dalla Barba, G. (1999). Confabulation and temporality. ashland, OH: Hogrefe & Huber Publishers. Dalla Barba, G., Boisse, M.-F., Bartolomeo, PAGS., & Bachoud-Levi, A.-C. (1997). Confabulation following rupture of posterior communicating artery: Cortex Vol 33(3) Sep 1997, 563-570. dalla Barba, G., Cipolotti, L., & Denes, G. (1990). Autobiographical memory loss and confabulation in Korsakoff's syndrome: Un informe de caso: Cortex Vol 26(4) Dic 1990, 525-534. Dalla Barba, G., Mantovan, M. C., Cappelletti, J. Y., & Denes, G. (1998). Temporal gradient in confabulation: Cortex Vol 34(3) Jun 1998, 417-426. Dalla Barba, G., Nedjam, Z., & Dubois, B. (1999). Confabulation, executive functions, and source memory in Alzheimer's disease: Cognitive Neuropsychology Vol 16(3-5) May-Jul 1999, 385-398. Dalla Barba, G., & Seassau, M. (2007). "Confabulation in dementia: Constantly compensating memory systems": Comentario: Neuro-Psychoanalysis Vol 9(1) 2007, 22-25. Dayus, B., & van den Broek, M. D. (2000). Treatment of stable delusional confabulations using self-monitoring training: Neuropsychological Rehabilitation Vol 10(4) Ago 2000, 415-427. DeLuca, J. (1993). Predicting neurobehavioral patterns following anterior communicating artery aneurysm: Cortex Vol 29(4) Dic 1993, 639-647. DeLuca, J. (2000). A cognitive neuroscience perspective on confabulation: Neuro-Psychoanalysis Vol 2(2) 2000, 119- 132. DeLuca, J. (2000). "A cognitive neuroscience perspective on confabulation": Response to commentaries: Neuro-Psychoanalysis Vol 2(2) 2000, 167-170. DeLuca, J. (2004). comentario sobre "The Pleasantness of False Beliefs": Neuro-Psychoanalysis Vol 6(1) 2004, 20-22. Deluca, J. (2005). Confabulation: A matter of philosophy of the mind? : Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society Vol 11(7) Nov 2005, 931. DeLuca, J. (2007). "Confabulation in dementia: Constantly compensating memory systems": Comentario: Neuro-Psychoanalysis Vol 9(1) 2007, 25-27. DeLuca, J., & Cicerone, K. D. (1991). Confabulation following aneurysm of the anterior communicating artery: Cortex Vol 27(3) Sep 1991, 417-423. DeLuca, J., & Diamante, B. J. (1995). Aneurysm of the anterior communicating artery: A review of neuroanatomical and neuropsychological sequelae: Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology Vol 17(1) Feb 1995, 100-121. Demery, J. Un., Hanlon, R. E., & Bauer, R. M. (2001). Profound amnesia and confabulation following traumatic brain injury: Neurocase Vol 7(4) 2001, 295-301. Downes, J. J., & Mayes, Un. R. (1995). How bad memories can sometimes lead to fantastic beliefs and strange visions. Malden, MAMÁ: Publicaciones de Blackwell. Dunning, D., & Popa, L. B. (1992). Examining the generality of eyewitness hypermnesia: A close look at time delay and question type: Psicología Cognitiva Aplicada Vol. 6(7) Dic 1992, 643-657. Feinberg, T. E. (2004). comentario sobre "The Pleasantness of False Beliefs": Neuro-Psychoanalysis Vol 6(1) 2004, 22-26. Feinberg, T. E., & Giacino, J. T. (2006). Confabulation. Cambridge, MAMÁ: La prensa del MIT. Feinberg, T. E., Roane, D. M., Kwan, P. C., Schindler, R. J., & et al. (1994). Anosognosia and visuoverbal confabulation: Archivos de neurología vol. 51(5) Mayo 1994, 468-473. Fischer, R. S., Alexander, M. PAGS., D'Esposito, M., & Otto, R. (1995). Neuropsychological and neuroanatomical correlates of confabulation: Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology Vol 17(1) Feb 1995, 20-28. Fotopoulou, Un., Conway, M., Griffiths, PAGS., Birchall, D., & Tyrer, S. (2007). Self-enhancing confabulation: Revisiting the motivational hypothesis: Neurocase Vol 13(1) Feb 2007, 6-15. Fotopoulou, Un., & Conway, M. Un. (2004). comentario sobre "The Pleasantness of False Beliefs": Neuro-Psychoanalysis Vol 6(1) 2004, 26-33. Fotopoulou, Un., Conway, M. Un., & Solms, M. (2007). Confabulation: Motivated reality monitoring: Neuropsicología Vol. 45(10) 2007, 2180-2190. Fotopoulou, Un., Solms, M., & Turnbull, O. (2004). Wishful reality distortions in confabulation: Un informe de caso: Neuropsicología Vol. 42(6) 2004, 727-744. Frost, PAGS., Lacroix, D., & Sanborn, N. (2003). Increasing false recognition rates with confirmatory feedback: A phenomenological analysis: Revista americana de psicología vol. 116(4) Victoria 2003, 515-525. Fujii, T., Suzuki, M., Suzuki, K., Ohtake, H., Tsukiura, T., & Miura, R. (2005). Normal memory and no confabulation after extensive damage to the orbitofrontal cortex: Diario de Neurología, Neurocirugía & Psiquiatría vol. 76(9) Sep 2005, 1309-1310. Gainotti, G. (1975). Confabulation of denial in senile dementia: An experimental design: Psychiatria Clinica Vol 8(3) 1975, 99-108. Garven, S., Madera, J. M., Malpass, R. S., & Shaw, J. S., III. (1998). More than suggestion: The effect of interviewing techniques from the McMartin Preschool case: Revista de psicología aplicada vol. 83(3) Jun 1998, 347-359. Gilboa, Un., Alain, C., Stuss, D. T., Melo, B., Molinero, S., & Moscovitch, M. (2006). Mechanisms of spontaneous confabulations: A strategic retrieval account: Cerebro: A Journal of Neurology Vol 129(6) Jun 2006, 1399-1414. Graff-Radford, N. (2000). "A cognitive neuroscience perspective on confabulation": Comentario: Neuro-Psychoanalysis Vol 2(2) 2000, 148-150. Gudjonson, G. H., & Clare, Yo. C. H. (1995). The relationship between confabulation and intellectual ability, memoria, interrogative suggestibility and acquiescence: Personalidad y diferencias individuales vol. 19(3) Sep 1995, 333-338. Gudjonson, G. H., & Sigurdsson, J. F. (1995). The relationship of confabulation to the memory, intelligence, suggestibility, and personality of juvenile offenders: Revista nórdica de psiquiatría vol. 49(5) 1995, 373-378. Gudjonson, G. H., & Sigurdsson, J. F. (1996). The relationship of confabulation to the memory, intelligence, suggestibility and personality of prison inmates: Psicología Cognitiva Aplicada Vol. 10(1) Feb 1996, 85-92. Gundogar, D., & Demirci, S. (2006). Multiple sclerosis presenting with fantastic confabulation: Hospital General de Psiquiatría Vol 28(5) Sep-Oct 2006, 448-451. Hanba, J. M., & Zaragoza, M. S. (2007). Interviewer feedback in repeated interviews involving forced confabulation: Psicología Cognitiva Aplicada Vol. 21(4) Mayo 2007, 433-455. Hays, J. R., Emmons, J., & Stallings, G. (2000). Dementia and mental retardation markers on the Rey 15-item Visual Memory Test: Informes psicológicos vol. 86(1) Feb 2000, 179-182. Hicks, L., & Myslobodsky, M. (1997). Mnemopoesis: Memories that wish themselves to be recalled? Hillsdale, NJ, Inglaterra: Lawrence Erlbaum Asociados, Inc. Hirstein, William (2005), Brain Fiction: Self-deception and the riddle of confabulation, Cambridge, Massachusetts: Prensa del MIT, ISBN 0-262-08338-8, HTTP://books.google.com/books?id=_rkKxbevFZEC&printsec=frontcover&dq=brain+fiction+self-deception+and+the+riddle+of+confabulation&hl=en&sa=X&ei=hjZqT_24L-GdmQWB_8z_CA&ved=0CDEQ6AEwAA, recuperado el 21 Marzo 2012 Hirstein, W. (2005). Brain fiction: Self-deception and the riddle of confabulation. Cambridge, MAMÁ: Prensa del MIT. Hoermann, S. (2007). "Confabulation in dementia: Constantly compensating memory systems": Comentario: Neuro-Psychoanalysis Vol 9(1) 2007, 28-31. Holliday, R. E., & Albon, Un. J. (2004). Minimising Misinformation Effects in Young Children with Cognitive Interview Mnemonics: Psicología Cognitiva Aplicada Vol. 18(3) Apr 2004, 263-281. jacob, C., & Pfuhlmann, B. (2003). Confabulatory Paraphrenia: Krankenhauspsychiatrie Vol 14(2) Jun 2003, 76-78. Johnson, M. K. (1991). Reality monitoring: Evidence from confabulation in organic brain disease patients. Nueva York, NY: Oxford University Press. Johnson, M. K. (2000). "A cognitive neuroscience perspective on confabulation": Comentario: Neuro-Psychoanalysis Vol 2(2) 2000, 150-158. Johnson, M. K., Hayes, S. M., D'Esposito, M., & Raye, C. L. (2000). Confabulation. Amsterdam, Países Bajos: Elsevier Science Publishers B V. Johnson, M. K., O'Connor, M., & Cantor, J. (1997). Confabulation, memory deficits, and frontal dysfunction: Cerebro y Cognición Vol. 34(2) Jul 1997, 189-206. Johnson, M. K., & Raye, C. L. (1998). False memories and confabulation: Trends in Cognitive Sciences Vol 2(4) Apr 1998, 137-145. Jorn, M. L., & Rybarczyk, B. (1995). Interpreting the confabulations of geriatric medical inpatients: Dos casos de estudio: Gerontólogo Clínico Vol. 16(1) 1995, 59-62. Joseph, R. (1984). Two brains, one child: Interhemispheric information transfer deficits and confabulatory responding in children aged 3-10: Resúmenes de disertación internacional. Joseph, R. (1986). Confabulation and delusional denial: Frontal lobe and lateralized influences: Revista de Psicología Clínica Vol 42(3) Mayo 1986, 507-520. Joseph, R., Gallagher, R. E., Holloway, W., & Kahn, J. (1984). Two brains, one child: Interhemispheric information transfer deficits and confabulatory responding in children aged 4, 7, 10: Cortex Vol 20(3) Sep 1984, 317-331. Joslyn, D., Grundvig, J. L., & Chamberlain, C. J. (1978). Predicting confabulation from the Graham-Kendall Memory-For-Designs Test: Revista de Consultoría y Psicología Clínica Vol. 46(1) Feb 1978, 181-182. Karlin, R. Un. (1983). Forensic hypnosis--two case reports: A brief communication: International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis Vol 31(4) Oct 1983, 227-234. El bolso, S. M., & Kiechel, K. L. (1996). The social psychology of false confessions: Compliance, internalization, and confabulation: Ciencia Psicológica Vol. 7(3) Mayo 1996, 125-128. Kern, R. S. (1990). Neuropsychological assessment of confabulation in Alzheimer's disease: Resúmenes de disertación internacional. Kern, R. S., Van Gorp, W. G., Cummings, J. L., Marrón, W. S., & et al. (1992). Confabulation in Alzheimer's disease: Cerebro y Cognición Vol. 19(2) Jul 1992, 172-182. Kerns, L. L. (1986). Falsifications in the psychiatric history: A differential diagnosis: Psiquiatría: Journal for the Study of Interpersonal Processes Vol 49(1) Feb 1986, 13-17. Kinsbourne, M. (2000). The mechanism of confabulation: Comment on "A cognitive neuroscience perspective on confabulation." Neuro-Psychoanalysis Vol 2(2) 2000, 158-162. Kinsbourne, M. (2004). comentario sobre "The Pleasantness of False Beliefs": Neuro-Psychoanalysis Vol 6(1) 2004, 33-37. Kleiger, J. H., & Peebles-Kleiger, M. J. (1993). Toward a conceptual understanding of the deviant response in the Comprehensive Rorschach System: Revista de evaluación de la personalidad vol. 60(1) Feb 1993, 74-90. Koehler, K., & jacoby, C. (1978). Acute confabulatory psychosis: A rare form of unipolar mania? : Acta psiquiátrica Scandinavica vol. 57(5) Mayo 1978, 415-425. Kopelman, M. D. (1987). Two types of confabulation: Diario de Neurología, Neurocirugía & Psiquiatría vol. 50(11) Nov 1987, 1482-1487. Kopelman, M. D. (1997). Anomalies of autobiographical memory: Amnesia retrógrada, confabulation, delusional memory, psychogenic amnesia, and false memories. Nueva York, NY: Prensa Plenum. Kopelman, M. D., Guinan, E. M., & Lewis, P. D. R. (1995). Delusional memory, confabulation and frontal lobe dysfunction: A case study in De Clerambault's syndrome: Neurocase Vol 1(1) 1995, 71-77. Kopelman, M. D., Ng, N., & Van Den Brouke, O. (1997). Confabulation extending across episodic, personal, and general semantic memory: Cognitive Neuropsychology Vol 14(5) 1997, 683-712. Kramer, S., bryan, K. L., & Frith, C. D. (1998). 'Confabulation' in narrative discourse by schizophrenic patients: International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders Vol 33(Suppl) 1998, 202-207. Laurence, J.-R., & Sidra de pera, C. (1983). Forensic hypnosis in the late nineteenth century: International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis Vol 31(4) Oct 1983, 266-283. Sotavento, E., Akanuma, K., Meguro, M., Ishii, H., Yamaguchi, S., & Meguro, K. (2007). Confabulations in remembering past and planning future are associated with psychiatric symptoms in Alzheimer's disease: Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology Vol 22(8) Nov 2007, 949-956. Sotavento, E., Meguro, K., Hashimoto, R., Meguro, M., Ishii, H., Yamaguchi, S., et al. (2007). Confabulations in Episodic Memory Are Associated With Delusions in Alzheimer's Disease: Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology Vol 20(1) Estropear 2007, 34-40. Lorente-Rovira, E., Pomarol-Clotet, E., McCarthy, R. Un., Berríos, G. E., & McKenna, P. J. (2007). Confabulation in schizophrenia and its relationship to clinical and neuropsychological features of the disorder: Medicina Psicológica Vol. 37(10) Oct 2007, 1403-1412. Lu, L. H., Barrett, Un. M., Schwartz, R. L., Cibula, J. E., Gilmore, R. L., Uthman, B. M., et al. (1997). Anosognosia and confabulation during the Wada test: Neurología vol. 49(5) Nov 1997, 1316-1322. Manning, L., & Kartsounis, L. D. (1993). Confabulations related to tacit awareness in visual neglect: Behavioural Neurology Vol 6(4) Victoria 1993, 211-213. Mattioli, F., Miozzo, Un., & Vignolo, L. Un. (1999). Confabulation and delusional misidentification: A four year follow-up study: Cortex Vol 35(3) Jun 1999, 413-422. Merckelbach, H., Muris, PAGS., Horselenberg, R., & Stougie, S. (2000). Dissociative experiences, response bias, and fantasy proneness in college students: Personalidad y diferencias individuales vol. 28(1) Ene 2000, 49-58. Metcalf, K., Langdon, R., & Coltheart, M. (2007). Models of confabulation: A critical review and a new framework: Cognitive Neuropsychology Vol 24(1) Feb 2007, 23-47. Moscovitch, M. (1989). Confabulation and the frontal systems: Strategic versus associative retrieval in neuropsychological theories of memory. Hillsdale, NJ, Inglaterra: Lawrence Erlbaum Asociados, Inc. Moscovitch, M. (1995). Confabulation. Cambridge, MAMÁ: Prensa de la Universidad de Harvard. Moscovitch, M., & Melo, B. (1997). Strategic retrieval and the frontal lobes: Evidence from confabulation and amnesia: Neuropsicología Vol. 35(7) Jul 1997, 1017-1034. Mott, T. Un. (2000). Confabulation and self-monitoring in memory. Resúmenes de disertación internacional: Sección B: Las Ciencias y la Ingeniería. Moulin, C. J. Un., Conway, M. Un., Thompson, R. G., Jaime, N., & Jones, R. W. (2005). Disordered memory awareness: Recollective confabulation in two cases of persistent deja vecu: Neuropsicología Vol. 43(9) 2005, 1362-1378. Nathaniel-James, D. Un., & Frith, C. D. (1996). Confabulation in schizophrenia: Evidence of a new form? : Medicina Psicológica Vol. 26(2) Estropear 1996, 391-399. Nedjam, Z., Barba, G. D., & Pillon, B. (2000). Confabulation in a patient with fronto-temporal dementia and a patient with Alzheimer's disease: Cortex Vol 36(4) Sep 2000, 561-577. Neressian, E. (2000). "A cognitive neuroscience perspective on confabulation": Comentario: Neuro-Psychoanalysis Vol 2(2) 2000, 163-166. Nys, G. M. S., van Zandvoort, M. J. E., Roks, G., Kappelle, L. J., de Kort, P. L. M., & de Haan, E. H. F. (2004). The Role of Executive Functioning in Spontaneous Confabulation: Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology Vol 17(4) Dic 2004, 213-218. Pantel, J. (2006). Review of Brain Fiction: Self-Deception and the Riddle of Confabulation: Revista Americana de Psiquiatría Vol 163(3) Estropear 2006, 559. Papagno, C., & Baddeley, Un. (1997). Confabulation in a dysexecutive patient: Implication for models of retrieval: Cortex Vol 33(4) Dic 1997, 743-752. Pezdek, K., Sperry, K., & Owens, S. M. (2007). Interviewing witnesses: The effect of forced confabulation on event memory: Ley y comportamiento humano vol. 31(5) Oct 2007, 463-478. Pfefferbaum, B., Allen, J. R., Lindsey, E. D., & Whittlesey, S. W. (1999). Fabricated trauma exposure: An analysis of cognitive, conductual, and emotional factors: Psiquiatría: Procesos Interpersonales y Biológicos Vol. 62(4) Victoria 1999, 293-302. Pihan, H., Gutbrod, K., baas, U., & Schnider, Un. (2004). Dopamine inhibition and the adaptation of behavior to ongoing reality: Neuroinforme: Para la comunicación rápida de la investigación en neurociencia, vol. 15(4) Estropear 2004, 709-712. Pisani, Un., Marra, C., & Silveri, M. C. (2000). Anatomical and psychological mechanism of reduplicative misidentification syndromes: Neurological Sciences Vol 21(5) 2000, 324-328. Ptak, R., Birtoli, B., Imboden, H., Hauser, C., Weis, J., & Schnider, Un. (2001). Hypothalamic amnesia with spontaneous confabulations: A clinicopathologic study: Neurología vol. 56(11) Jun 2001, 1597-1600. Ptak, R., & Schnider, Un. (1999). Spontaneous confabulations after orbitofrontal damage: The role of temporal context confusion and self-monitoring: Neurocase Vol 5(3) 1999, 243-250. Redlich, Un. D. (2000). False confessions: The influence of age, suggestibility, and maturity. Resúmenes de disertación internacional: Sección B: Las Ciencias y la Ingeniería. Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (2000). Distortions of memory. Nueva York, NY: Oxford University Press. Roos, C., & Gow, K. (2007). The effect of emotional arousal on recall and interrogative suggestibility: Australian Journal of Clinical & Experimental Hypnosis Vol 35(2) Nov 2007, 150-168. Sacos, Oliver (1985), El hombre que confundió a su mujer con un sombrero, Nueva York: Perennial Library, ISBN 0-06-097079-0, Archived from the original on 8 Abril 2011, HTTP://web.archive.org/web/20110408211740/http://www.arvindguptatoys.com/arvindgupta/wife-hat.pdf, recuperado el 21 Marzo 2012 Salazar-Fraile, J., Tabares-Seisdedos, R., Selva-Vera, G., Balanza-Martinez, V., Martinez-Aran, Un., Catalan, J., et al. (2006). "Recall and Recognition Confabulation in Psychotic and Bipolar Disorders: Evidence for Two Different types Without Unitary Mechanisms": Errata: Psiquiatría Integral Vol. 47(2) Mar-Abr 2006, 159. Salazar-Fraile, J., Tabares-Seisdedos, R., Selva-Vera, G., Balanza-Martinez, V., Martinez-Arn, Un., Catalan, J., et al. (2004). Recall and Recognition Confabulation in Psychotic and Bipolar Disorders: Evidence for Two Different Types Without Unitary Mechanisms: Psiquiatría Integral Vol. 45(4) Navidad-agosto 2004, 281-288. Sandson, J., Albert, M. L., & Alexander, M. P. (1986). Confabulation in aphasia: Cortex Vol 22(4) Dic 1986, 621-626. Schacter, D. L., & Curran, T. (1995). The cognitive neuroscience of false memories: Anales psiquiátricos vol. 25(12) Dic 1995, 726-730. Schnider, Un. (2000). Spontaneous confabulations, disorientation, and the processing of 'now': Neuropsicología Vol. 38(2) 2000, 175-185. Schnider, Un. (2001). Spontaneous confabulation, reality monitoring, and the limbic system--a review: Brain Research Reviews Vol 36(2-3) Oct 2001, 150-160. Schnider, Un. (2003). Spontaneous Confabulation and the Adaptation of Thought to Ongoing Reality: Nature Reviews Neuroscience Vol 4(8) Ago 2003, 662-671. Schnider, Un. (2004). comentario sobre "The Pleasantness of False Beliefs" Armin Schnider: Neuro-Psychoanalysis Vol 6(1) 2004, 37-39. Schnider, Un., Bonvallat, J., Emond, H., & Leemann, B. (2005). Reality confusion in spontaneous confabulation: Neurología vol. 65(7) Oct 2005, 1117-1119. Schnider, Un., Gutbrod, K., Hess, C. W., & Schroth, G. (1996). Memory without context: Amnesia with confabulations after infarction of the right capsular genu: Diario de Neurología, Neurocirugía & Psiquiatría vol. 61(2) Ago 1996, 186-193. Schnider, Un., & Ptak, R. (1999). Spontaneous confabulators fail to suppress currently irrelevant memory traces: Nature Neuroscience Vol 2(7) Jul 1999, 677-681. Schnider, Un., Ptak, R., von Daniken, C., & Remonda, L. (2000). Recovery from spontaneous confabulations parallels recovery of temporal confusion in memory: Neurología vol. 55(1) Jul 2000, 74-83. Schnider, Un., Treyer, V., & Buck, Un. (2000). Selection of currently relevant memories by the human posterior medial orbitofrontal cortex: Revista de neurociencia vol. 20(15) Ago 2000, 5880-5884. Schnider, Un., von Daniken, C., & Gutbrod, K. (1996). The mechanisms of spontaneous and provoked confabulations: Cerebro: A Journal of Neurology Vol 119(4) Ago 1996, 1365-1375. Servan, J., Verstichel, PAGS., Catala, M., & Rancurel, G. (1994). Confabulation and amnesia in posterior cerebral infarcts: Revue Neurologique Vol 150(3) 1994, 201-208. Shallice, T. (1999). The origin of confabulations: Nature Neuroscience Vol 2(7) Jul 1999, 588-590. Shapiro, B. E. (1983). Hemispheric specialization of higher cortical functions in the human brain: Resúmenes de disertación internacional. Shapiro, L. R., & Purdy, T. L. (2005). Suggestibility and Source Monitoring Errors: Blame the Interview Style, Interviewer Consistency, and the Child's Personality: Psicología Cognitiva Aplicada Vol. 19(4) Mayo 2005, 489-506. Shelton, C. (2004). Progression of Memory/Attention Abilities Post-Traumatic Brain Injury: Journal of Cognitive Rehabilitation Vol 22(1) Spr 2004, 5-9. Sheng-chun, J., Kai, W., & Xiao-si, L. (2005). Development of Researches on Confabulation: Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology Vol 13(4) Nov 2005, 492-494. Sigurdsson, E., Gudjonson, G. H., Kolbeinsson, H., & Petursson, H. (1994). The effects of electroconvulsive therapy and depression on confabulation, memory processing, and suggestibility: Revista nórdica de psiquiatría vol. 48(6) 1994, 443-451. simpson, J., & Done, D. J. (2002). Elasticity and confabulation in schizophrenic delusions: Medicina Psicológica Vol. 32(3) Apr 2002, 451-458. Sjoberg, R. L. (2000). The catechism effect: Child testimonies during a 17th-century witch panic as related to educational achievement: Memory Vol 8(2) Estropear 2000, 65-69. Herrero, PAGS., & Gudjonson, G. H. (1995). Confabulation among forensic inpatients and its relationship with memory, suggestibility, compliance, ansiedad, and self-esteem: Personalidad y diferencias individuales vol. 19(4) Oct 1995, 517-523. Solms, M. (2000). A psychoanalytic perspective on confabulation: Neuro-Psychoanalysis Vol 2(2) 2000, 133-138. Sporer, S. L. (2004). Reality monitoring and detection of deception. Nueva York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Stuss, D. T., Alexander, M. PAGS., Lieberman, Un., & Levine, H. (1978). An extraordinary form of confabulation: Neurología vol. 28(1) Nov 1978, 1166-1172. Suelves, J. M. (2005). Confabulation: Gaining Understanding of Human Nature From Neurological Disorders: PsycCRITIQUES Vol 50 (39), 2005. Tallberg, I.-M. (2001). Deictic disturbances after right hemisphere stroke: Revista de pragmática vol. 33(8) Ago 2001, 1309-1327. Tallberg, I.-M. (2007). Confabulation in dementia: Constantly compensating memory systems: Neuro-Psychoanalysis Vol 9(1) 2007, 5-17. Tallberg, I.-M. (2007). "Confabulation in dementia: Constantly compensating memory systems": Respuesta: Neuro-Psychoanalysis Vol 9(1) 2007, 32-40. Tallberg, I.-M., & Almkvist, O. (2001). Confabulation and memory in patients with Alzheimer's disease: Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology Vol 23(2) Apr 2001, 172-184. Turnbull, O. (2004). "The Pleasantness of False Beliefs: An Emotion-based Account of Confabulation": Comment reply: Neuro-Psychoanalysis Vol 6(1) 2004, 39-45. Turnbull, O. H., Berry, H., & Evans, C. E. Y. (2004). A positive emotional bias in confabulatory false beliefs about place: Cerebro y Cognición Vol. 55(3) Ago 2004, 490-494. Turnbull, O. H., Jenkins, S., & Rowley, M. L. (2004). The Pleasantness of False Beliefs: An Emotion-based Account of Confabulation: Neuro-Psychoanalysis Vol 6(1) 2004, 5-16. Weinstein, E. Un. (1971). Linguistic aspects of amnesia and confabulation: Revista de Investigación Psiquiátrica Vol 8(3-4) Ago 1971, 439-444. Weinstein, E. Un. (1996). Symbolic aspects of confabulation following brain injury: Influence of premorbid personality: Boletín de la Clínica Menninger Vol. 60(3) Suma 1996, 331-350. Weinstein, E. Un., & Kahn, R. L. (1955). Reduplication. Springfield, IL: Editorial Charles C. Thomas. Welch, L. W., Nimmerrichter, Un., Gilliland, R., Rey, D. E., & Avión, P. R. (1997). "Wineglass" confabulations among brain-damaged alcoholics on the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised Visual Reproduction Subtest: Cortex Vol 33(3) Sep 1997, 543-551. Pozos, G. L., & Bradfield, Un. L. (1998). "Good, you identified the suspect": Feedback to eyewitnesses distorts their reports of the witnessing experience: Revista de psicología aplicada vol. 83(3) Jun 1998, 360-376. Whitlock, F. Un. (1981). Some observations on the meaning of confabulation: British Journal of Medical Psychology Vol 54(3) Sep 1981, 213-218. Zaragoza, M. S., Payment, K. E., Ackil, J. K., Drivdahl, S. B., & Beck, M. (2001). Interviewing witnesses: Forced confabulation and confirmatory feedback increase false memories: Ciencia Psicológica Vol. 12(6) Nov 2001, 473-477. Esta página utiliza contenido con licencia Creative Commons de Wikipedia (ver autores).

Si quieres conocer otros artículos parecidos a Confabulation puedes visitar la categoría Pages with reference errors.

Deja una respuesta

Tu dirección de correo electrónico no será publicada. Los campos obligatorios están marcados con *

Subir

we use own and third party cookies to improve user experience More information